Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-24T09:58:09.961Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Recognizing the Bank Robber and Spotting the Difference: Emotional State and Global vs. Local Attentional Set

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 May 2014

Antonia Pilar Pacheco-Unguetti*
Affiliation:
Universidad de las Islas Baleares (Spain)
Alberto Acosta
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
Juan Lupiáñez
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to A. P. Pacheco Unguetti, PhD. Department of Psychology. Universidad de las Islas Baleares. Ctra de Valldemossa, Km 7.5. 07122. Palma de Mallorca (Spain). E-Mail: ap.pacheco@uib.es

Abstract

In two experiments (161 participants in total), we investigated how current mood influences processing styles (global vs. local). Participants watched a video of a bank robbery before receiving a positive, negative or neutral induction, and they performed two tasks: a face-recognition task about the bank robber as global processing measure, and a spot-the-difference task using neutral pictures (Experiment-1) or emotional scenes (Experiment-2) as local processing measure. Results showed that positive mood induction favoured a global processing style, enhancing participants’ ability to correctly identify a face even when they watched the video before the mood-induction. This shows that, besides influencing encoding processes, mood state can be also related to retrieval processes. On the contrary, negative mood induction enhanced a local processing style, making easier and faster the detection of differences between nearly identical pictures, independently of their valence. This dissociation supports the hypothesis that current mood modulates processing through activation of different cognitive styles.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 249255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700001193 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Positive affect and flexibility: Overcoming the precedence of global over local processing of visual information. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 123134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-005-7957-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darling, S., Martin, D., Hellmann, J. H., & Memon, A. (2009). Some witnesses are better than others. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 369373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.010 Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: Trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 745761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00117-2 Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In Niedenthal, P. M. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (167196). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 107117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.2.107 Google Scholar
Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7, 181185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00354.x Google Scholar
Dodson, C. S., Johnson, M. K., & Schooler, J. W. (1997). The verbal overshadowing effect: Why descriptions impair face recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25, 129139. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03201107 Google Scholar
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0047707 Google Scholar
Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, M., & Tanaka, J. M. (1998). What is “special” about face perception? Psychological Review, 105, 482498. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.482 Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). The value of positive emotions: The emerging science of positive psychology is coming to understand why it’s good to feel good. American Scientist, 91, 330335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1511/2003.4.330 Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000238 Google Scholar
Gao, Z., Flevaris, A. V., Robertson, L. C., & Bentin, S. (2011). Priming global and local processing of composite faces: revisiting the processing-bias effect on face perception. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , 73, 14771486. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0109-7 Google Scholar
Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 3440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00406 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granholm, E., Cadenhead, K., Shafer, K. M., & Filoteo, J. (2002). Lateralized perceptual organization deficits on the global-local task in schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 4252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.1.42 Google Scholar
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0 Google Scholar
Hunsinger, M., Isbell, L. M., & Clore, G. L. (2012). Sometimes happy people focus on the trees and sad people focus on the forest: Context-dependent effects of mood in impression formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 220232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211424166 Google Scholar
Huntsinger, J. R. (2012). Does positive affect broaden and negative affect narrow attentional scope? A new answer to an old question. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 595600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027709 Google Scholar
Huntsinger, J. R., Clore, G., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and global-local focus: Priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing. Emotion, 10, 722726. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019356 Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60415-3 Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions. (2 nd Ed., pp. 417435). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2005). “We all look the same to me”: Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 875881. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01631.x Google Scholar
Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of holistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 2438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.24 Google Scholar
Kimchi, R., & Palmer, S. E. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 521535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.521 Google Scholar
Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Purkhart, R., Ries, J., Olié, J. P., & van der Meer, E. (2011). Visuo-spatial cognition in schizophrenia: Confirmation of a preference for local information processing. Schizophrenia Research, 127, 163170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.027 Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Digitized photographs, instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. B. (2006). Eye-witnesses should not do cryptic crosswords prior to identity parades. Perception, 35, 14331436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p5666 Google Scholar
Macrae, C. N., & Lewis, H. L. (2002). Do I Know you? Processing orientation and face recognition. Psychological Science, 13, 194196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00436 Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3 Google Scholar
Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Callejas, A., & Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Attention and Anxiety: Different attentional functioning under state and trait anxiety. Psychological Science, 21, 298304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359624 Google Scholar
Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Lupiáñez, J., Román, N., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Response inhibition and attentional control in anxiety. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 646660. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.637114 Google Scholar
Robertson, L. C., & Lamb, M. R. (1991). Neuropsychological contribution to theories of part/whole organization. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 299330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(91)90012-D Google Scholar
Sanz, J. (2001). Un instrumento para evaluar la eficacia de los procedimientos de inducción de estado de ánimo: “La escala de valoración del estado de ánimo” (EVEA) [An instrument to assess mood induction procedures: The “Mood Evaluation Scale” (EVEA)]. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 27, 71110.Google Scholar
Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 3671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(90)90003-M Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. C., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Srinivasan, N., & Hanif, A. (2010). Global-happy and local-sad: Perceptual processing affects emotion identification. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 10621069. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930903101103 Google Scholar
Van Belle, G., de Graef, P., Verfaillie, K., Busigny, T., & Rossion, B. (2010). Whole not hole: Expert face recognition requires holistic perception. Neuropsychologia, 48, 26202629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.034 Google Scholar
Vila, J., Sánchez, M., Ramírez, I., Fernández, M., Cobos, P., Rodríguez, S., … Moltó, J. (2001). El Sistema Internacional de Imágenes Afectivas (IAPS): Adaptación Española (Part 2) [The International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Spanish adaptation]. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 54, 635657.Google Scholar
Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 8799. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9021-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yovel, I., Revelle, W., & Mineka, S. (2005). Who sees trees before forest? The obsessive-compulsive style of visual attention. Psychological Science, 16, 123129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00792.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zadra, J. R., & Clore, G. L. (2011). Emotion and perception: The role of affective information. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2, 676685. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.147 Google Scholar